A case of alleged cutting-up murder in Sweden: legal consequences of public outrage  

hhs

Lennart Sjöberg (1)
Stockholm School of Economics and Foundation for Forensic Psychology Danderyd, Sweden

Abstract

The paper reviews a case of probable murder of a prostitute in Stockholm in 1984. Two MD’s were tried in the case on the basis of several kinds of evidence. Most sensational, the daughter of one of the doctors, at the time barely more than 1 year old, was claimed to have been present at the murder, the cutting-up of the body and cannibalistic acts, and also herself subjected to sexual abuse by her father and his friend. All this is said to have been revealed in stories she told her mother several years later. The doctors were acquitted from the murder and sexual abuse charges but the District Court officially stated that they had been found to be guilty of the cutting-up of the body. The acquittal gave rise to strong outrage and was said to show how the ruling male elite protected their own and gave them a right to prey on women and children. On this basis, the licenses of the doctors to practice were later revoked in a complex set of legal decisions and the two doctors were, and are, severely ostracized. The case became a showcase for the feminist movement in Sweden and may have contributed to a lowering of the standards used by the courts for assessing the evidence necessary for convicting suspects of incest or other types of sexual abuse of children. The case is used as a basis for suggesting principles or hypotheses for further research on human error in forensic settings.

Introduction

In the summer of 1984, a total of six garbage bags containing parts of a woman’s body were found in Stockholm, on two different occasions. The woman could be identified by means of finger prints. It was found that she had been a prostitute and drug addict, 28 years old Catrine , who had disappeared some time before. The cause of death could never be established, too much of the body was missing (including the head which was never found). Also, the exact time of death could not be precisely determined on medical grounds and there was considerable confusion among some witnesses as to when they last heard anything from her.

The case is known in Swedish as ”styckmordet”, roughly translated as the cutting-up murder, the term I will use in the present paper. The case gave rise to intense interest in Sweden and it exemplifies many important forensic psychological and sociological problems and processes. It is the purpose of the present article to summarize the case for international readers and to discuss it in the light of what is known more generally about the dynamics of witness psychology and court decision making, and also to relate it to the wider social context.

The present paper thus gives a detailed account of a complex Swedish case, involving alleged murder. The crime took place in 1984 and the case has since then been processed in Swedish courts. At the time of writing (December 2001) final decisions have been made by Swedish courts but the case may be brought up in the European Court, a process also likely to take several years.

So far, the paper presents a clinical case study. An attempt will be made, however, to apply a broader perspective as well. The two most common approaches to research in forensic psychology are actuarial and clinical. Both have their drawbacks (Melton, Petrila, Poythress, & Slobogin, 1997). The actuarial method calls for data on a sizable number of cases which tend to be described in relatively abstract terms, and usually provide little of the contextual complexities that have arisen in each case. Clinical descriptions, on the other hand, are rich in detail but fewer, and it is often hard to know what knowledge of general value they produce. A third approach, pragmatic (Fishman, 1999), attempts to combine the two.

The case of the cutting-up murder has intrinsic interest due to many dramatic and unusual circumstances, but to warrant attention from international readers, a clinical case study is probably insufficient. In the present paper, such a case study will be presented, and a concluding section will deal with what can be learned from the case more generally. This is by necessity a section which is somewhat speculative, but if it can contribute to further theorizing it has served its purpose. It should be added that the discourse is predominantly psychological, although the wider social and historical context is briefly treated as well. Representatives of other disciplines would undoubtedly have chosen other perspectives and perhaps a very different story would have emerged. However, when things are seen from the standpoint of individuals and their ways of functioning, the result is likely to be psychological. An interesting illustration is provided by the insightful discussion of the investigation of the murder of Prime Minister Olof Palme, by two political scientists (Stern & Hansén, 2001).

 

The case

Overview

The discovery of the dead woman got much attention in the media, but there were no definite results from the police investigation. The reason was in part that other events required much of the resources available to the Stockholm Police, especially the murder of Prime Minister Olof Palme in the beginning of 1986. However, after several years, two MD’s were prosecuted for the alleged murder, and acquitted. Yet, the court stated they had been found guilty of the cutting up of Catrine’s body. Their licenses to practice medicine were later revoked by administrative court decisions. They had, and still do, all the time pleaded innocent. It is probable that the public thought they were guilty, as probably the court had when trying the murder case, but that they had to be acquitted due to a lack of solid enough proof. However, Swedish society had turned against them, and a very strong public opinion demanded that at the very least, their licenses to practice medicine should be revoked.

The case was to become one of the most discussed murder cases in Sweden in the decade to follow, second only to the assassination of Prime Minister Palme. It has recently again become a focus of debate, much due to a very thorough investigation of the case by Per Lindeberg, who published a lengthy book about it in the beginning of 1999 (Lindeberg, 1999). In addition, a translation to English of one of the most important testimonies, by witness psychologist Astrid Holgerson (Holgerson & Hellbom, 1997) has recently been published. Somewhat earlier, Scharnberg had published a thorough and critical examination of the case (Scharnberg, 1996b) in English. Another important document (in Swedish), a book by Hanna Olsson originally published in 1990, is being reprinted (Olsson, 1990). The case was appealed anew to the Supreme Administrative Court and the Supreme Court in 1999 and 2000.

Even to-day, the case gives rise to very strong feelings in Sweden. The very publication of Lindeberg’s book turned out to be controversial and a senior member of the publishing firm quit her job in protest (or under the threat of dismissal, according to the publisher’s version of the story). The manuscript had been referred by her to Hanna Olsson, whose book, mentioned above, was subjected by Lindeberg to serious scrutiny. Perhaps naturally, Olsson did not feel that Lindeberg’s book was worthy of publication.

Lindeberg’s book met with very mixed reactions, some of them very positive and some very negative. The book was given very extensive coverage in the newspapers and also on radio and television when it was published in the beginning of 1999. A member of the Swedish Academy, professor Knut Ahnlund, published a highly positive review (Ahnlund, 1999), and so did Jan Guillou, who is a leading journalist in Sweden and well-known author (Guillou, 1999). Others were quite negative. Leading newspaper Dagens Nyheter published, on a full page, a detailed and highly critical article. In May 1999, this 750 pages long book had sold about 3500 copies, including the whole of the first printing, a remarkable number for a Swedish book of this type and size.

I now turn to a summary of highlights of this complex case. For a more complete exposition, the reader is referred to Lindeberg’s book. The development of the case proceeded in several distinct phases.

 

Phase 1: The post-mortem dissector and his wife’s death

A young MD worked at the Department of Forensic Medicine of the Karolinska Institute in Stockholm (henceforth referred to as the Department). His name was Teet (2). He was to be the first and prime suspect in the case, already in the fall of 1984.

However, well before that some police officers had been suspicious of him, and rumors were at large. His wife had committed suicide – or was it murder? – in 1982. The suicide had some unusual aspects to it (death by hanging to the side of a bed, party dress clothing of the deceased), but the official investigation had concluded that it was doubtless a true suicide and nothing else. Later, further analysis was to confirm that conclusion. Yet, police officers at the time of the suicide noted what they saw as unusual, callous, reactions in Teet. In addition, he had insisted on publishing a paper on death by hanging, in spite of advice against it, shortly after his wife’s death. The rumors that he had murdered his wife were also spread by his previous father-in-law, who had never accepted that his daughter had killed herself. And similar rumors were spread by his previous mother-in-law, who also happened to be employed by the then leading Swedish tabloid called Expressen, a newspaper which was to play a very important role in the later campaign against Teet.

Teet was an unusual person in several senses. He took a very strong interest in his work, invited people to watch his autopsies and would even send them unsolicited autopsy reports. He had some interest in horror movies with sexual violence in them (3) and lived an uninhibited sexual life and had contacts with prostitutes (uncertain how many). His first wife was likewise sexually ”liberated” and also bisexual. At the time of her death, a divorce had already been decided upon and Teet had established a new permanent relationship. Some of all this was known to people at the Department. Although none of it was illegal, it was partly a potential problem for the Department, especially the contacts with prostitutes which would have been considered to be very embarrassing to a Department of Forensic Medicine – in fact ground for dismissal or at least refusal to renew a contract of employment (Teet had only a time limited contract running until the end of 1985).

 

Phase 2: Catrine’s body found, preliminary investigation

The body of Catrine was found in two separate bags, at different occasions, fairly close to the Department of Forensic Medicine. Identification was first impossible but later possible with the aid of fingerprints (her hands were found in one of the bags). A senior member of the Department (Jovan Rajs) concluded his official assessment of the case with murder as the probable cause of death. He also stated that the cutting-up of the body was at least partially done in a very professional manner – partly clearly not (4). Later, the same expert, now promoted to the rank of Full Professor, had to agree, when testifying in court in 1988, that the cause of death clearly could not be assessed on the basis of the evidence available to him. In particular, the missing head made such conclusions impossible (5). He did agree, however, to the prosecutor’s assertion that cutting-up of bodies almost always had been preceded by murder. On the other hand, only a handful of cases like this had taken place in Sweden in modern times (Rajs, Lundström, Broberg, Lidberg, & Lindquist, 1998), so the basis of that conclusion was not very extensive, provided that only Swedish data were to be seen as relevant. Most commentators have since concluded that murder was not legally established, but there are exceptions.

One legal scholar still argues that the cutting-up of a body is always preceded by murder and hence proof of murder (Diesen, 1997). Although it is certainly very reasonable to assume that a cutting-up of a corpse and its disposal was preceded by murder, one wonders if it is proof of murder in the legal sense. There could of course be several reasons for attempts to hide a corpse, not all of them tied to murder. On the other hand, there are different types of cutting-up and in the Catrine case the cutting-up did not appear to be wholly rationally calculated and only caused by an intention to hide evidence. Evidence of the case suggests that it was a case of sadistic, sexually motivated dismembering of the woman’s body. The fact that her remains were left where they were likely to be found and identified could express a wish by the perpetrator(s) to experience the thrill of publicity and subsequent police investigations.

Teet was arrested in December of 1984 both for suspicions related to the death of his wife two years earlier and for the new case of Catrine. He was held for questioning by the police for 5 days which was the legal maximum. He denied having murdered the two women. In fact, he denied having ever met Catrine. He admitted reluctantly to a few contacts with prostitutes in Stockholm. His home was searched but no incriminating evidence was found. In particular, no objects belonging to Catrine were found, no letters or phone numbers to her and no prescriptions written to Catrine by Teet. (Other doctors had written many prescriptions to Catrine, but not the two who were to become suspects in the case, as far as the police could establish).

The Catrine case and the suspicions against a specialist of forensic medicine made for large headlines in the press, and not only in the tabloids. It was easy to find out the identity of the suspect. It sems as if the police were eager that his identity should not be protected, as is usually the case until a suspect is convicted and even after that. The reason was possibly that they wanted to make sure that he must be fired from his job; his alleged frequent contacts with prostitutes was very troubling to the police. He was also fired from his job since the Board of Social Welfare, his ultimate employer, could not accept a staff member who was suspected of such serious crimes, and it turned out to be very difficult to find an alternative placement for him. Under the pressure of these events, Teet tried to take his life but was saved. However, as a side effect of his treatment he lost most of his hearing.

In the summer of 1986, the case was dismissed by the police for lack of any solid evidence against Teet and any further clues. The closure the case was very little attended to by the same media who had made a lot of profit on scandalizing Teet in the fall. His life was now in ruins, but much worse was to come.

 

Phase 3: The child and the second doctor

Teet had a student in the beginning of the 1980’s, called Thomas . At one occasion he had helped Thomas to fulfill a course obligation he had missed by being absent from an autopsy. In gratitude, Thomas and his wife invited the Teet couple for dinner. This was in 1982, after the death of Teet’s wife and when Teet had established a relationship with a new woman, Monica (fictive name).

The dinner did not go very well, largely because Monica came dressed in a very provocative manner and especially Thomas wife resented this. They were not to meet socially again, and Thomas wife took an intense dislike to Teet. When she read, in the fall of 1984, that a post-mortem dissector was being suspected for the alleged cutting-up murder of Catrine, she called the police and asked if it was Teet. Amazingly, the police officer confirmed her suspicion. At that very point in time, she was extremely upset about her suspicions that her husband had sexually abused their daughter, who had been born in the beginning of 1983. The child had some physical signs that could perhaps be interpreted to that effect by a lay person and she had been examined several times. The doctors had assured Thomas wife that there was no ground for her suspicions, but she did not drop them. Due to these suspicions and other problems the Thomas marriage had been breaking up for some time. A divorce was to follow.

Now, some 2.5 years after Catrine’s death an amazing process began. The child made some off-hand remarks, such as about a doll having no head. The mother got the idea that maybe her husband, who knew Teet, had been Teet’s accomplice and maybe they had brought the child to witness the murder and cutting-up of Catrine! She started to question her daughter very extensively, and made notes and some tape recordings. More and more incriminating statements were, according to the mother, made by the child. She finally submitted a 200 pages long summation of what the child had allegedly said to the police. She also made very frequent calls to the police as soon as she felt that some new evidence had occurred in the child’s sayings.

The value of this evidence was assessed by a psychiatrist, Frank Lindblad, and a psychologist, Margareta Erixon. Their assessment has since been translated to English and can be found as an appendix in Holgerson (Holgerson & Hellbom, 1997). It is a loose ”psychodynamic and holistic” type of assessment concluding that the mother is absolutely credible, and that the child had actually visited the scene of the crime. For example, the lack of an emotional reaction in the child when taken to the Department of Forensic Medicine (where it was believed that the crime had taken place) was interpreted as solid evidence of her subduing her true underlying strong emotional reaction. This type of ”logic” allows, of course, virtually any observations to be offered as evidence of a hypothesis. It should be noted that the psychodynamic approach enjoys a very large credibility in Swedish society, at least among many members of the medical and forensic professions, and that the academic credentials of doctors and professors may go a very long way to establishing the credibility of even the most outrageous statements. Media seldom question this type of reasoning.

Yet, the legal value of the Child’s Story, as it came to be known, was very doubtful, as Holgerson pointed out in her thorough assessment of it (Holgerson & Hellbom, 1997). It is hearsay throughout. The child was not interrogated by the police or their experts. The mother did all the questioning and summarized it in her own words. She monitored the child to making statements which would incriminate her former husband and his ”accomplice”.

The following is a revealing statement by the mother:

I have drawn conclusions of what Britta has said. And I have woven pictures of what can have happened. And I have sometimes, so to say, been half a step ahead of Britta and sort of understood what she was aiming at now and then. And she has eventually arrived there herself. (p. 88, as translated by Holgerson).

The critical analysis by Holgerson is also supported by later research on conversational memory (Bruck, Ceci, & Francoeur, 1999). Mothers were found by Bruck et al. to have quite imperfect memory of what their children had said. Just what a child says and means to say is often very hard to analyze (Hellblom Sjögren, 1997), not to mention to remember.

The critical analysis by Holgerson was not made until the very last court proceedings in 1991, however, and up to that point the Child’s Story went more or less unchallenged. It made, of course, for a very attractive theme in the media and the assessment by Lindblad and Erixon, claimed to be fully accredited experts on child psychology and forensic psychiatry, seemed to be a guarantee that the story was entirely credible in all its grisly details – including ritual sexual abuse and cannibalism. Of course, two doctors guilty of such acts deserved severe punishment! And how could the words of the experts be questioned?

 

Phase 4: The photoshop

The Child´s Story mentioned a photographer and many people involved in the case believed that photos had been taken of the cutting-up and the parts of the dead body. However, no such photos were ever found.

But, at a late stage in the investigations (in the fall of 1987), a married couple who owned a photo shop close to the Department contacted the police. They said that years before, in the summer of 1984, they had developed and processed prints of a film roll which contained horrible images of a cutting-up. Many parts of the body were shown on these horrible pictures, and probably also a person with bloody clothes who apparently had taken part in the cutting-up. They, and their personnel (6), had been very upset by these pictures, they said, but all material had been given to the customer who had claimed that the pictures were part of a secret investigation and that they must tell nobody about them.

The photo shop owners were shown lineups with the two doctors and they identified Thomas and, to some extent, Teet. However, the written protocol described their identification as much more certain than it had in fact been. A scrutiny of tapes shows that:

! Teet was identified only by the wife, and then only after much hesitation. It was unclear if she identified Teet as the person who had been to their shop, or the person on one of the pictures. She seems to have been guided to this answer by reactions from the police officer present. Her first choice was another person. The husband wrongly pointed out one of the police officers as having been to their shop.

! Thomas was finally identified by both wife and husband as the customer in their shop, but after much hesitation.

The lineup was conducted in an unprofessional manner (Holgerson & Hellbom, 1997). The police officers who led it were aware of who the suspect was. In addition, in the first confrontation, with the post-mortem dissector, there were great physical differences between him and all the others, with one exception. Also, the others were all police officers. At the second confrontation, Thomas was distinguished from the others by his smiling expression. None of the other 7 men smiled but had a very neutral expression. All these facts break with principles suggested by the British Devlin commission (Devlin report, 1976), see also Goodman et al. (Goodman et al., 1999). The large effects of more or less subtle suggestions and monitoring by the police officer in charge of a lineup identification test have been amply documented (Cutler & Penrod, 1995).

In spite of the low quality of the lineup and the alleged identification of the MD’s by the photo-shop owners, the Administrative Court of Appeal cited this evidence as the main support for the guilt of the suspects, and hence a very important justification for revoking their licenses.

Research on lineups has shown that they are unreliable. Levi estimated conservatively that the probability of being innocent when chosen in a lineup was 0.247 (Levi, 1997). Since the court is supposed to acquit a defendant whenever there is reasonable doubt about his or her guilt, conviction would seem to be quite appropriate if the only or main evidence cited is that of a lineup identification. The present case has features well known in research on lineups, such as exaggerated belief in their efficiency (Levi & Jungman, 1995), and the ineffectiveness of a defense counsel present at the lineup (Stinson, Davenport, Cutler, & Kravitz, 1996). People tend to be overconfident (Lichtenstein, Fischhoff, & Phillips, 1982); such overconfidence being possibly to some extent a characteristic of the individual (Bornstein & Zickafoose, 1999). Recent research has made investigators more and more alerted to the risks of mistaken identification on the basis of lineups (Wells et al., 1998). Wells and Bradfield (Wells & Bradfield, 1999) recently wrote:

There is increasing evidence that mistaken eyewitness identifications from lineups and photo spreads are the most frequent cause of juries convicting the wrong person. Analyses of cases in which people were mistakenly convicted by juries have shown eyewitness misidentification to account for more cases than all other causes of wrongful conviction combined… (p. 138).

 

The two trials

Chief Prosecutor Anders Helin, who was handling the case, was uncertain, in spite of the Child’s Story and the photoshop owners’ testimony, if the evidence was strong enough to justify prosecution of the two MD’s. However, he went ahead and brought them to trial, and the District Court of Stockholm tried the case early in 1988. The two doctors were suspected not only of incest, murder and cutting-up of the body of Catrine, but also of ritual sadistic abuse (although probably not of the satanistic kind) and even cannibalism, all on the basis of statements the mother claimed that her daughter had made. They were also suspected of having drugged the child. Formally, the doctors were accused of murder and child sexual abuse, while the cutting-up of the dead woman’s body was a minor offense, and the period for prosecution of it had expired. Naturally, the case was given an enormous amount of media coverage.

Several of the lay members of the court agreed to be interviewed in the media before the final sentence was decided, expressing confusion and uncertainty, yet willingness to convict for murder, and the chairperson of the court had been absent from a crucial meeting. A mistrial was therefore declared and the Appeals Court, in a highly unusual move, instructed the District Court that a conviction for murder would probably not be upheld in the Appeals Court. At this point, Chief Prosecutor Helin was inclined to drop the case, but the case was now given new political impetus by an article in Dagens Nyheter, Sweden’s leading daily newspaper, by psychotherapist Hanna Olsson. Olsson claimed that the suspects were not treated as they deserved but protected by a male oppressive society which did not even want to listen to testimony by the oppressed female prostitutes at the very bottom of society. (It was true that Helin had said that he wanted to avoid such witnesses, probably because he considered them unreliable and of little or no help. Whether that was a correct judgement can be debated but since these women often were drug users it could have been at least partly justified. Some deragatory statements made by Helin to media, on the other hand, were not).

The second trial, with a new court, acquitted the suspects from murder and child abuse, but also stated that they were guilty of the cutting-up of Catrine’s body. This was a remarkable move since they had not been formally accused of the cutting-up. And since the court had not formally sentenced them for the cutting-up, just stated in writing that the court was convinced they had done it, they could not appeal! In the eyes of the public they had, of course, been convicted of the cutting-up and even Chief Prosecutor Helin (now retired) still, at the time of writing, officially claims that so was the case.

A decision to revoke their licenses to practice was finally made by the Administrative Court of Appeal in 1991 and the final court of appeal, the Supreme Administrative Court, refused to hear the case, making the decision final (7).

The judicial trickery used by the District Court (8) in fact deprived the suspects of their legal right of a full judicial assessment of their guilt – and even if the crime formally was a minor one and the period for prosecution had expired it was ground for their continued ostracization and marginalization in society and for the revocation of their licenses to practice their profession. These were no small matters to them, and the economic compensation awarded to Teet (some SEK 300,000 or US $ 35,000) was a trifle compared to the consequences that were to follow on the statement by the court. The two men are still ostracized and even people having anything to do with them have been known to be treated harshly. The mere mentioning of the name of Teet in an article used in the teaching of medical students in Stockholm gave rise to outrage and hysterical headings in tabloids (9).

I now turn to a discussion of a number of themes that can be used to throw some light on this dramatic case.

 

Social perspectives

Understanding outrage

The case gave rise to an almost unprecedented public outrage in Sweden. It had several aspects that make it easy to understand this reaction, even if it could perhaps not have

been predicted. The brutal and possibly sadistic treatment of the woman’s body was surely one such aspect. Almost everyone also assumed she had been murdered and humiliated by being subjected to perverted sexual demands. She was also seen as a victim in the sense of having lived a life at the very bottom of society, and having had her children taken from her by the social authorities. The little girl who many believed had also been abused and made to witness the grisly events further added to outrage. The suspicion that the perpetrators were two high status members of the elite of society, and indeed two MD’s who are supposed to care about people and help them, not abuse and murder them, added further fuel to the fire already burning.

Suspicions were widely held that abuse of women and children was a very large social problem, and the case illustrated, to many, the dangers of a fairly common male inclination towards indiscriminate and violent sexual behavior which in turn, in their view, had the function of expressing and supporting the power relations in a male dominated society. The treatment of the case in the courts, ending in acquittal from the murder charge, appeared to further support these views.

In addition, the case developed according to a dramaturgic logic of very unusual intensity and efficiency. The evidence against the suspects, of quite varying quality, came forth in several phases and over a long period of time. The case tended therefore to rise anew in the media, over and over again. The outcome was also very uncertain since the courts did not agree on the final treatment of the suspects and their – so far – final fate was sealed only after 7 years. The media were of course very dedicated to covering the case from all possible angles. The assassination of the Prime Minister and all the events following that historic event took place during the same time period and attracted even more media attention, but the cutting-up murder competed successfully for a large share of media attention.

The book by Olsson (Olsson, 1990), entitled ”Catrine and Justice” (my translation), mentioned above, played an important role in the events and should therefore be discussed in some detail. It was published at a late stage in the process, but before the final decisions in the administrative courts regarding the two doctors’ licenses to practice medicine. It is a relatively brief text (215 pages) and quite well written, and it conveys a deeply felt outrage by its author. She was clearly convinced that the suspects were guilty of murder and the other crimes they were indicted for, and from her point of view, they got away with it. And the reader of her book is similarly convinced, if he or she does not check the facts any further. Olsson depicts all evidence against the suspects as persuasive and reliable, while arguments in their favor are subjected to detailed criticism and then dismissed. She assumes the role of the prosecutor. Some of her arguments do carry weight, such as her criticism of the intervention by the Court of Appeals, and her arguments favoring that homicide had very likely preceded the cutting-up. However, at times she gave a misleading picture of the evidential value of testimony in the case, as when she stated that ”The photo shop owners were very sure about the statements they gave” (p. 104). As we have seen, this is not at all what came out of a reading of the protocol from their responses to the lineups they were exposed to. There was much uncertainty and some mistaken identifications involved.

Olsson’s interpretation of the events is that the ”system” protected its own, e. g. that some doctors were eager to support their suspected colleagues and that even the prosecutor was very reluctant to bring up important evidence because he did not want to ”smear” the men. Chief Prosecutor Helin actually did make such a statement. Also, the Council of Forensic Medicine could have formulated a better argument than they did. They dismissed the murder suspicions on the logically correct ground that the cause of death could not be established but the other possible causes of death they mentioned were even less supported by any evidence, and the logic of the case, as well as the meager previous experience of similar cases, did reasonably lead to the conclusion of homicide. But still, Olsson did not admit or even discuss that there was valid doubt about much of the evidence in the case and also great difficulty for a court to convict for murder in a case where the cause of death was still in doubt. A further legal complication of great importance is that two suspects were indicted and that, even if their guilt had been established, it would probably have been impossible to determine who had committed the murder. In such a case, acquittal was prescribed by Swedish law.

 

The media

The media clearly played a very central role in the events, and the way they functioned is not very flattering to the standards of media integrity. The case was clearly seen by media as having a great commercial interest, but that is no excuse for misleading media reports. The privacy of the suspects was not honored; their identity could very easily be found out on the basis of media information.

The media were very gullible when it came to the matter of ”the Child’s Story”. This ”story” was never anything more than summaries made by the child’s mother, summaries of alleged conversations between her and her daughter.

Since media had so gravely misled the public about the amount and quality of evidence against the two doctors, it is no wonder that the acquittal brought about a very angry reaction from many quarters. The prosecutor was also strongly criticized for not having invited testimony by the several prostitutes who claimed that they had known Teet as a customer, that they knew he had been a customer of Catrine, or even seen him together with Catrine (10). The way the trial was conducted was therefore seen as an example of male oppression of women, and the fact that the two doctors were acquitted was said to be a denial of justice to Catrine.

The media treatment of the case was not very balanced or factually correct, with a few exceptions. The media tended to take for granted that the suspects were guilty of heinous crimes: to recapitulate, very frequent adultery with prostitutes, ritualistic abuse of at least one of these and a child, homosexual intercourse (not a crime but morally condemned), cutting-up of the murdered woman’s body and eating parts of it, all this in the full view of a child aged about a year. The ”evidence” of all these crimes was stories by prostitutes who tended to talk about what they heard others had said about abuse, but some of them probably had been suppliers of sex services to the post-mortem dissector. Further evidence cited were the Child’s Story as told to the mother, some witnesses (especially the photoshop owners) and forensic evidence from the dead body. Few critical analyses of all this were available, and what was pubslished tended to be vehemently attacked and even smeared.

The strong aggression against the skeptics in some media, and the media siding with the prosecutor and police, warrant some discussion. Media in Sweden are often quite skeptical about the police and have by no means usually accepted police or official versions of other famous crimes. In particular, the contemporary attempts of the police and prosecutors to establish credibility of their explanation of the murder of Prime Minister Palme (first it was a group of alleged terrorists, then a ”lone maniac”) has largely failed and the media give much space to other speculations (11). However, the media tend to take an ”underdog” perspective and in the case of the cutting-up murder the suspects were members of the elite. This very fact made the case much more sensational and salient to media than if the suspect had been, say, a Polish butcher who could well have become a prime suspect in the case (12). The media were certainly not trying to protect two members of the male elite – on the contrary these two persons were given a very harsh treatment and enormous media exposure to great detriment to their lives.

The strongest reactions were directed against the Forensic Council (13) of the National Board of Health and Welfare whose members had found Rajs’s assessment of the cutting-up evidence wanting. The statement made by the council was quite sober and factual but it was depicted as an unjustified and slanderous attack on Rajs by some media. The council statement was even to be all but ignored in the final sentence by the Administrative Court of Appeal in 1991. A leading witness psychologist, Holgerson, was subjected to some very negative and contemptuous comments in media when she had concluded that the Child’s Story was not credible evidence. It may be noted that some media, among them even important quality newspapers, had asserted that the story had been told by the child to expert child psychologists and psychiatrists, something which was simply not true.

 

The courts and the government

In all fairness, it must be said that the legal system was by no means as gullible as the media. As we have seen, it took strong pressure to make Chief Prosecutor Helin re-introduce the case after the declaration of a mistrial in 1988. He had available, at that point, all the evidence that was to be used by the courts and some of it was very persuasive to the media and the lay public. Indeed, his very reluctance and his unwillingness to hear in court some of the several drug addicts/prostitutes who had provided information about links between Teet and Catrine, was to be the subject of outrage and very strong criticisms by Hanna Olsson and other feminist opinion leaders. As we have noted, the Court of Appeal had stated, in their unusual intervention (14), that they found it very unlikely that a murder conviction would be upheld. And the question of the revocation of the licenses to practice medicine of the suspects was to be treated by the courts and administrative bodies in several phases before a final decision was reached in 1991, also under considerable public opinion pressure.

Strong pressure had been exerted on the Supreme Administrative Court to take action and they finally instructed the Administrative Court of Appeal to do so, which was apparently inconsistent with their practice in cases of license revocation.

It is paradoxical that the interference by the Court of Appeals with the District Court trial was instrumental in depriving the suspects of their right of appeal. They were formally acquitted, as noted above, by the District Court, but the sentence was formulated in such a manner that they still were regarded as guilty. Only the prosecutor (15) could then have appealed the sentence. He did not, and has explained that, since the Appeals Court had already made a public statement in the case, he considered an appeal to be meaningless. However, he apparently did not consider that it was also his responsibility to guard the rights of the suspects, and an appeal would have given them a chance to question the conclusions in the District Court reluctant acquittal.

The political leadership of the country assumed a very low profile in the case. The trade union of medical doctors (unions are very important in Sweden and a very high percentage also of professionals are members) first defended the rights of the suspects to continue to practice medicine, once they had been acquitted of the crimes they were accused of, but this attitude of the union led to a split and very angry protests from a large group of female doctors.

 

Ineffective defense counsels

The credibility of the psychologists/psychiatrists is such that few attorneys apparently feel up to questioning them in court and the weakness of many defense counsels is only too obvious. Scharnberg has recently pointed to the defense counsels as a weak link in the Swedish system of justice (Scharnberg, 1998). In the case discussed in the present article, an employee of one of the counsels even stated in public her belief in the guilt of the accused.

The defense counsels tended to be ineffective, possibly to some extent because they were not prepared to carry out a critical analysis of some of the psychiatric evidence offered against their clients. Maybe they were also not really convinced that their clients were innocent – the whole story was ever present in the media and the media depicted the case in a manner very negative to the suspects.

 

The feminist and anti-incest movement

Olsson had intervened effectively with her articles and book on the feminist theme (Olsson, 1990) (16). She took it for granted that the suspects were guilty and depicted it as a scandal and oppression of women that they had been acquitted of murder. A strong opinion was then formed by feminist groups who filed many letters to courts and authorities where they demanded that the suspects should at least have their licenses to practice medicine revoked (Lindeberg, 1999). It is interesting to note that Olsson’s book was finished when the case was still pending and the final revocation of the licenses was highly uncertain, and that the 1994 re-print, which included a new preface by Catrine’s sister, did not mention what the final outcome of the case had been. The reader who only consults Olsson’s book is thus left in ignorance of the outcome and his or her outrage is not mitigated by the events that did take place after the book was finished.

The enormous interest and outrage connected with the case of the cutting-up murder must be seen in the context of rising feminism and incest outrage. In the 1980’s the previous sexual liberalism of the 1960’s and 1970’s was beginning to be replaced by a moralistic and puritanistic attitude, perhaps under the influence of the AIDS epidemic (17). But how should incest outrage be explained? It can be viewed in the context of the changed roles of men and women and the weakened status of the family, changes which took place in the 1970’s and the 1980’s. Women no longer stayed at home and kept a constant watch over their children, they had to leave them to daycare centers to pursue their own professional careers. The role of a homemaker was frowned upon in contempt and economically harshly punished by new tax legislation. These momentous changes of long standing norms could have given rise to feelings of guilt and anxiety among women, in particular, over the fate of their children.

At this point in time, the anti-incest movement was at a high point and had still not been subjected to the critical analysis which was to come later in the 1990’s (Öhrström, 1996). Many people believed that incest was a very large problem and that a very large share of all children, especially girls, were at risk. Many also believed that incest could be diagnosed by psychologists and psychiatrists on the basis of tests and behavior observations, that there were ”symptoms” of incest.

Even accusations about satanistic ritual abuse and organized cults were beginning to appear in Scandinavia, as usual under the influence of events in the USA. There are, of course, by now many stories of satanistic incest cults which have been rejected as untrue (La Fontaine, 1998). In Sweden, there have been a few cases. In Norway, there was the case of Bjugn where a small community was tormented by widespread accusations starting in a pre-school and quickly spreading to leading members of the community (Kringstad, 1997). Nobody was convicted in the Bjugn case but the County Board demonstrated its belief in the guilt of at least some of the suspects (or maybe only in one of them) by awarding financial compensation to the children. Some of the children in Bjugn, but not all, have later retracted their accusations, and the physical examinations which were the most solid base of evidence in the eyes of medical experts, that some abuse had occurred, are by now no longer considered credible by a unanimous expert opinion.

The Bjugn case illustrates how the administrative authorities, in contrast to the courts, are likely to believe in the truth of outrageous accusations. In the cutting-up murder case, the National Board of Health and Welfare even argued officially that the demands for solid evidence should be lowered in trying whether a doctor’s license should be revoked as compared to criminal proceedings. This suggestion was, however, rejected by the Supreme Administrative Court. As another example, the Union of Psychologists published a statement denouncing Holgerson’s contribution as a witness psychologist.

 

The rise of psychologists

Starting in the 1960’s, the traditional core family pattern of differentiated gender roles began to break up (Barnett & Hyde, 2001). Parenthood was something that could and should be taught by experts. Experts and specially educated staff of daycare centers were better equipped to take care of children than were parents. Nobel laureates Alva and Gunnar Myrdal, very influential Social Democrats, had delineated this movement and its ideology in a much discussed book already in the 1930’s (Myrdal & Myrdal, 1934). Of course, collectivistic approaches to child upbringing and the idea of ”social engineering” were at the core of these ideological beliefs, shared by many brands of socialism.

New and greatly expanded professional groups of psychologists, psychiatrists and social workers institutionalized the demands to be put upon parents, and these demands were not small, nor were they precise. The new breed of experts claimed that they could formulate the demands a parent must meet, such as ”see to the needs of the child”, ”defining boundaries”, avoiding something nebulously called a ”symbiotic relationship”, etc. The Government willingly and readily accepted the claims made for a scientific basis of such concepts and norms, and also awarded to psychologists, for the first time, official certification by the National Board of Health and Welfare in 1978. Hence, the Government officially accepted that the activities of psychologists were based on respectable and reliable science.

Similar claims made in the USA have been strongly criticized (Dawes, 1994; Hagen, 1997). Therapy in the sense of a ”talk cure” can probably be just as well delivered by an intelligent and mature person with no formal education in psychology or psychiatry (Strupp & Hadley, 1979). The diagnostic power of personality tests and clinical judgments in forensic applications has been challenged and little evidence in support of strong claims made by the clinical profession has been presented. Yet, the claims continue to be made, and continue to be believed (Sjöberg, 1998-99).

The new ”experts” were given great power as consultants in the courts and the social authorities, and their statements could easily decide the fate of families. The state was eager to intervene and children were taken, every year in the thousands, from their parents and put in foster homes. (This practice is still carried out). In the foster homes, they were rarely attended to by the authorities who seldom found complaints about such homes credible. And the power of the social authorities was such that they need not heed to court decisions. E.g., if a father was prosecuted for incest, tried and acquitted, his child (often all his children) would still have been taken by force from the family and placed in a foster home, and that decision would not be rescinded just because the suspicions against the father had turned out to be unfounded by a Court of Law. A few such cases were successfully brought to the European Court, but that is a very slow and uncertain process. Settlements between Sweden and the parents happened sometimes, and were quite cheap for the state, or the Swedish authorities just ignored European Court sentences

All of these cases were not cases of incest, but also of alleged neglect. The debate about the Swedish ”kinder-gulag” has been intense since the 1980’s when some cases were given wide publicity, e.g. the so-called Alexander case (Wolff, 1986). This case illustrated in a convincing manner how the social authorities stuck to their policy, even in the face of overwhelming evidence of their having made a mistake. Psychologists often play an important supportive role in such a process by investigating, often testing, the child and coming up with ”scientific” conclusions supporting interventions.

In one recent case, a father has been denied to see his daughter on the basis of flimsy ”evidence” partially consisting of testings of the girl with projective tests. One is a well-known American test, the Children’s Apperception Test, and the other a local variant, the so-called Erica Method, which builds upon observations of a child’s play. The latter method has no research foundation at all worth mentioning (Sjöberg, 1997, 2000). The former method has recently been object of very serious criticism (Knoff, 1998; Reinehr, 1998). The man, who is a senior medical researcher, appealed to the Swedish Psychological Association and asked about the scientific basis for these methods which, being used by one of its members, had been instrumental in destroying his life as he saw it. After a very long wait and many reminders, he was informed by the Association’s Chairperson about some research on the Rorschach test with adult patients. The Rorschach is rightly criticized for its doubtful validity, but even if it were to be granted that it has some validity, it would not be transferable to very different projective tests used for different purposes and with children, not adults. The case shows quite clearly that the Association embraces such test use as was practiced in this case, and also that it feels that there is no need for specific support of tests in the concrete applications they are put to.

 

Forensic psychological perspectives

The case will now be analyzed both from the standpoint of the judgments actually made and the more general background which probably affected all or most of the people making those judgments.

The case is quite complex and developed over a long period of time. There was no physical evidence against the two doctors, nor were there any witnesses to the alleged murder and cutting-up of the woman’s body. Opinions about the case differed widely, but it is fair to state that the majority opinion of the courts was that it was likely that the accused were guilty, although the evidence was not strong enough to convict them. The main evidence for their guilt was:

  • The child’s story as told to and reported by the mother
  • The line-up judgments made by the photoshop owners
  • In addition, especially Teet’s personality and behavior had some unusual aspects to them that made it seem quite possible or even likely that he was guilty.

These bases for inferring guilt are fragile. First, the child’s story was merely hearsay and it is very doubtful that a child can remember such events that took place very early in her life. Considerable research on child testimony has show that there is a danger that children are led, by adults, to make false accusations (Ceci & Bruck, 1995). In this case, the child’s mother had ample opportunity, over a prolonged period of time, to guide her daughter into constructing a false memory. It was also likely that the mother had a motive for doing so due both to her strong dislike and suspicions against Teet and the pending conflict with her former husband. This is not to say, of course, that the mother did not herself believe that her daughter=s memories were correct.

The line-up was conducted in a non-professional manner. It can not be excluded that the police officer in charge more or less unconsciously guided the witnesses to identify the two doctors. Also, the identification that was made was quite uncertain and not made consistently. It has later been found that illegal photographs of autopsies were by no means unique in the area (the photoshop was quite close to the Medical School) and the vivid memories that the photoshop owners had of gruesome pictures may have stemmed from quite a different source than the cutting-up of Catrine´s body.

The personality arguments, especially concerning Teet, may have contributed in an important way to the courts’ assessment of the case, even if they are peripheral in the official statements made by the courts. However, the facts that a person was a customer of prostitutes, showed quite a lot of interest in very violent movies, and took an intense interest in his work as a post-mortem dissector do not speak for his being a murderer. All the cited attributes of the man are, or were, by no means unique. What is unique is being a murderer. In Sweden, the homicide rate is very low, only about 300 cases per year in the whole country with a population of about 9 million. The probability that a person is a murderer is therefore exceedingly low. The base rate fallacy is well known (Tversky & Kahneman, 1974). People make probability judgments without taking due consideration of the base rate, and are much influenced by the representativeness. In this case, Teet may well have seemed to be a likely killer on the basis if the attributes cited, yet such an inference would have been quite misleading and unfounded.

The development of the case shows a strongly hypothesis-driven process. From the very start, the police officers in charge seem to have had grave suspicions about Teet due to the peculiar circumstances surrounding his first wife’s suicide. Also, they were annoyed and troubled by his being a customer of prostitutes which was considered to be unacceptable behavior by someone working in forensic medicine and hence in close cooperation with the police. Teet’s behavior also aroused annoyance and even suspicions in other people, especially Thomas wife. What we see here is the gradual build-up of Teet as a likely killer, and the media supported the process.

The media, in fact, displayed a common tendency to ”psychologize”. Several commentators found it easy to construct and contribute to the scenario of the two doctors being guilty as accused. Again, Teet’s unusual personality and behavior was fruitful material for constructing a scenario of guilt. The people of the courts were not unaffected by the media coverage, and besides they had probably themselves similar tendencies to construct scenarios and draw premature conclusions from them.

Finally, it is pertinent to inquire whether a different forensic psychological practice would have helped in the case and would have mitigated the wider implications of it. The answer is definitely yes. The initial analysis of the credibility of the child’s story was quite speculative and gullible, as shown above. The problem here seems to have been closely connected to the psychodynamic framework of the two consultants. Current research on human memory and suggestibility prescribes a much more skeptical attitude than they displayed.

In addition, what was known about eyewitness testimony and line-up identification errors should have prescribed a different practice in conducting the line-up or, since that did not happen, a skeptical attitude to the evidence it produced. In addition, it would have been valuable to conduct some research on base rates. For example, just how common was it for photoshops in the area to work with gruesome pictures of autopsies? The case was presented at the time as if such an event was completely unique but now it seems that it may not have been.

The literature on human judgment errors is vast. Some of the phenomena documented there, such as biased probability judgements due to heuristics, are quite relevant in the case. Such errors could have been made by both judges and the media and, indeed, even by defense counsels.

Could these errors have been avoided? Perhaps not, after all. In the final hearings by the Administrative Court in 1991, Holgerson and Hellbom forcefully criticized both the gullible report on the child’s story and the line-up identification. Yet, the two doctors had their licenses revoked. This may have been due to the courts affording more belief to the original report about the child’s story and the line-up results, or it could also have happened wholly or partly due to the very strong opinion pressure in the case.

 

The present situation of the case

In the wake of the cutting-up case, there have been many other cases in Sweden where suspects have been convicted on the basis of weak evidence. Just how many is not known, but there is ample enough documentation (Scharnberg, 1996a, 1996b; Öhrström, 1996). Scharnberg documents 50 cases. Media are strongly interested in reporting any news about sexual abuse of children and some cases where day-care personnel have been suspects have been given enormous publicity recently.

A small group of witness psychologists, working in a tradition established by Trankell (Trankell, 1972) had specialized in the critical scrutiny of evidence offered by witnesses, often children. Their activity was, and still is, highly controversial, as illustrated by strong emotional reactions to an excellent book on these themes by Hellblom Sjögren (Hellblom Sjögren, 1997; Sjöberg, 1998), exemplified in a book review by legal scholar Diesen (Diesen, 1997-98) in a leading scientific law journal in Sweden.

Lindeberg’s book about the case was published in January 1999 and was immediately given a very unusual amount of media attention. Several newspapers published not one, but several, reviews and debate articles about it, and the reactions were quite polarized. Some well known authors and journalists gave the book high praise and others condemned it as a reflection of ”contempt for women”. Some participants in the debate argued that even if the two MD’s were innocent of murder they should have their licenses revoked because they had committed perverse sexual acts. It may be added that license revocation, being a very serious event to those affected by it, is quite rare and requires very serious crimes. Being a customer of prostitutes is currently illegal in Sweden (18) but was not so in the 1980’s. Incest is of course a serious crime and was so also in the 1980’s but the charges at this point were not the important point in the legal proceedings leading to license revocation, and they were probably not seen as well substantiated (19).

In 1999, the two MD’s, filed an appeal to the Supreme Administrative Court in order to try to have their cases re-opened. Media also reported on some important new clues in the case which apparently had been brought to the attention of the police due to the publicity given to Lindeberg’s book. It was said that there is reliable testimony certifying that Catrine was alive well after the date when she was murdered according to the prosecutor’s statement in the 1988 trial (Annerud, 1999). In three articles published in June, 1999, in the leading tabloid Aftonbladet, Cantwell reported that the police were working very hard to find a new suspect and that they were convinced that the two doctors were innocent (Cantwell, 1999). The crucial testimony that Catrine was alive well after Whitsuntide 1984 was allegedly forgotten due to the tremendous pressure on the police after the murder of Prime Minister Olof Palme in 1986, and the subsequent personnel changes and re-allocation of resources. It also seems that a mistake was done in filing one of the key testimonies regarding Catrine’s alleged activities after the Whitsuntide holiday, leading to its dismissal. A thesis by two law students of the University of Stockholm (Styrlin & Nyberg, 1999) (20) is critical to the fact that the whereabouts of Catrine at that time were not investigated more carefully by police and prosecutor. Finally, Catrine is known to have injected narcotics of some kind on the Sunday before the alleged murder took place, but traces of a drug were not found at the autopsy to the extent necessary to confirm the ”official” day of death.

In 2001, both the Supreme Court and the Supreme Administrative Court denied the two doctors a new trial. According to them, the new evidence about another perpetrator was not strong enough, and the technical legal arguments about the District Court sentence were rejected on the ground that a sentence of acquittal could not be appealed. The doctors now consider bringing their case to the European Court.

 

 

Does the case contribute to forensic psychological knowledge?

How can something of general value and interest be learned from a case such as the present one? In my view, the best way to use the case is to deduce tentative principles or hypotheses that it suggests and illustrates.

Thus, six principles and topics for further research may be formulated on the basis of the case, and they may be of value in accumulating knowledge from this and other cases:

1. Stigmatizing those who behave in an unusual way. The process of stigmatization (Goffman, 1963) of the doctors took, as its starting point, the suspicions aroused in some key actors by the unusual behavior and personality of one of them. It is possible that deviating people are at risk when other factors are at play which demand that a culprit be found. Deviations can be quite innocent, yet a basis for ominous scenarios. Consider the case of preschool teacher Michaels (Bruck & Ceci, 1995) who was accused, and convicted, of severe sexual abuse of children. She may have acted in an unusual way at times (talking to herself). There is sometimes a small step, in the minds of many, from being a bit unusual to committing severe and perverse crimes. Hence it is hypothesized that deviant behavior, no matter if it is innocent in itself, may form a basis for the construction of ominous scenarios, if the opportunity arises to do so.

2. Neglect of base rates. The behavior of one of the doctors was unusual, but not that unusual. It was certainly within the boundaries of normality, which murder of course was not. The conclusion of murder involved highly unusual behavior, and the strength of the evidence was clearly below a reasonable verdict of guilty. Yet, many concluded that the doctors were guilty and their lives were ruined. Base rate neglect has been well documented in research on judgment and decision making (Kahneman & Tversky, 1984), and the phenomenon quite possibly has many potentially fruitful applications in forensic settings.

3. Premature closure. In spite of the relatively large resources spent on the case, some clues were neglected and investigations were concentrated against the two doctors. It appears that a feeling developed very early among prosecutors and police officers on the case, that the doctors were guilty. Nobody knows, of course, who was guilty but in order to increase quality of the investigation it would have been important to approach it with a more open mind. The situation is similar to the problem of deciding if a person is lying. This is more difficult than most people think (Vrij, 2000) but it can be achieved to some extent by keeping an open mind. The importance of expectations and made-up scenarios for a low quality investigation is an interesting hypothesis which is brought up by the circumstances of the case.

4. Rhetoric as a way of escaping from responsibility. The case does not show Swedish justice at its best. It was clear that the courts and authorities conceded more to public outrage than they should have done and what happened is still, in December 2001, a source of considerable embarrassment. Chief Prosecutor Helin claims to have forgotten mistakes he made, and can offer no explanation of them. The present Chief Prosecutor (Bjarne Rosén) refuses to re-consider the case with the argument that Athe doctors have been acquitted and they cannot be more acquitted than that (21). This is similar to the argument made by the Prosecutor-General in 1991 that the statement by the District Court that the doctors were guilty of the cutting-up could have no legal consequences and hence that the doctors should not be allowed a new trial. In both cases the rhetoric is quite cynical and misleading since the doctors have indeed suffered the consequences greatly. Rhetoric involves the shifting use of words in a more or less clever way, so as to persuade or at least silence somebody with embarrassing demands or claims. The use of shifting perspectives is common in discussions about various socially Ahot@ topics (Sjöberg & Montgomery, 1999). Its prevalence and particular uses in legal contexts constitutes an interesting topic.

5. Exaggerated belief in recovered memories. The present case is in one sense only one of many where recall of events, in this case by a child, has been attributed what is probably grossly exaggerated credibility. The child=s story was produced under wholly uncontrolled circumstances and by her mother, not by a professional. The two experts that did testify to the credibility of the story stepped on very thin ice indeed. It should, in all fairness, be added that this was in 1984 and the whole modern debate about recovered memories had hardly begun. It is possible that a the child=s story would have met with more skepticism to-day, but it is not certain. Gudjonsson found indications of skepticism about recovered memories in a recent study in the UK (Gudjonsson, 1997).

6. Folk psychology in the courts. The problem of recovered memories is just one example of the power of intuitive, or folk, psychology notions. It just seems to be very credible that memories can be repressed, and later recovered in great and dramatic detail. Maybe it is less likely to the layman that such accounts can be wholly false when created under the influence of an authority figure such as a psychologist, a police officer or a parent. What makes a testimony about remembered events credible? How do judges, prosecutors, police officers and defense counsels reach their conclusions about the matter? Granhag and Strömwall recently carried out a survey and found that members of the legal professions entertained quite erroneous notions about what constitutes a credible memory report (Granhag & Strömwall, in press). The notions they have are probably quite important for the conclusions they reach, and hence central to the functioning of the courts. The topic is in urgent need of further research.

 

Conclusion

The cutting-up murder case, second in importance of Swedish homicide cases in the 20th century, appeared on the media agenda in the middle of the 1980’s when incest and child abuse concern was at its peak. Its many grisly details made for excellent media sales, and honest outrage was felt and expressed by many people over the alleged crimes of murder, child abuse and cannibalism. There is nothing to be said against such outrage, on the contrary it was justified given that the media story and the prosecution’s case against the suspects was true and proven. However, that was hardly the case. The evidence boiled down to the highly questionable ”Child’s Story” – supported only by a low quality psychological analysis – and line-up evidence which also was very uncertain. No physical evidence was brought forward. The suspects all the time claimed they were innocent. The time contingencies alone made it quite unlikely that the murder could have been carried out as stated by the prosecutor. On the other hand, there was at least one other suspect which seemed to be much more likely as the perpetrator (see note 12). On top of all this, the legal system worked in unusual and highly questionable ways in this particular case, probably to a large extent because of the public sentiment against the suspects. The final outcome of the District Court trial, acquittal with stigmatization combined with later license revocation, was disastrous for the suspects and could not be appealed due to formalities.

Forensic psychological work of higher quality throughout the case could possibly have contributed to a more reasonable and just process. It would have been most important to have a higher quality process from the very beginning since the case involved a build-up over a prolonged period of time, when expectations were formed which led to new expectations. It may be quite difficult to change the direction of a process once it has started in a certain direction. In the present case, there was some very good forensic psychological work, but it came in the very last phase when opinions about guilt were probably quite difficult to change – in addition to the tremendous public outrage which demanded action against the doctors.

 

1. I am grateful to professor Robyn Dawes, Dr. Astrid Holgerson and Per Lindeberg for their comments on the manuscript.

2. Only the forenames of the two doctors and the victim are given in this edition of the paper.

3. This theme was rather prominent in media, and some of the videos were shown in court. It was probably unavoidable that the ”snuff movie” urban legend also was to be invoked, implying an interest in movies showing real murder of people. No such movies have ever been documented and the legend can be traced to a clever producer in the 70’s who made quite an economic success out of spreading this rumour about his movie (Stine, 1999). What Teet saw was ”splatter movies”, i.e. violent movies with no claim to show real events.

4. It was to be argued that the perpetrator was a professional and also very clever, and wanted to confuse the police so he made some cuts look like he was unprofessional. Or – a less common argument – two people had been involved in the cutting-up of the body. What type of ”professional” knowledge and experience was at stake here was not very clear. Maybe a butcher or a hunter could have done it as well as a specialized MD.

5. A missing head might indicate some professional knowledge on the part of the perpetrator and his intent to make it impossible to establish the cause of death. On the other hand, the fact that hands were found shows that the perpetrator did not care about trying to hide the identity of the victim. The perpetrator(s) also did not care about trying to conceal her remnants as such since they had been disposed of in nature in such a way that they would surely be found.

6. No other members of the staff of the photoshop, who also allegedly had seen the pictures and reacted strongly to them according to the shop owners, could remember anything about these pictures.

7. In most cases a first appeal is always granted in the Swedish system, while a second appeal is very seldom granted and then only for reasons of general legal significance.

8. Recent statements by one of the members of the court attribute the outcome to a combination of pressure both from time and probably from public outrage against the suspects, and from a need to compromise since some members of the court wanted to convict the suspects for murder.

9. Teet had co-authored an article about rape. When informed about this, a tabloid ran a headline stating that ”the post-mortem dissector is now teaching students how to rape women at the Medical School”.

10. The latter claim also came from two police officers but has later been shown to be false (Lindeberg, 1999).

11. It may be noted that the only trial for the murder of Olof Palme, so far, took place in 1989 and that some of the key actors in the cutting-up murder case were involved also in the Palme murder trial. The suspect – a ”lone maniac” – was convicted by the District Court, which was however not unanimous, and acquitted by the Appeals Court. In the latter court, witness psychologist Astrid Holgerson played a crucial role in critizing the testimony by the chief eye witness, Palme’s wife, who also happened to be a psychologist. Chief Prosecutor Helin was also involved in both the Palme murder case and the cutting-up case.

12. This man, who died in 1988, had already committed several murders in Sweden, one of them a cutting-up case, had no alibi and had been seen among Stockholm prostitutes at the time when Catrine disappeared. He was very dangerous, especially when under the influence of alcohol, but the police dismissed him as a suspect at a very early stage, on no known factual grounds. They had their eyes on Teet from the very beginning.

13. Its members were three senior specialists in forensic medicine and two senior judges.

14. This intervention was to be strongly criticized by the Attorney General (in Swedish ”Justititiekanslern” who is not a cabinet member) about a year later. This office is in charge of assessing any complaints about the legality of decisions made by authorities, including courts, and they seldom find any fault with those authorities.

15. To be fully correct, the family members could also have appealed. Thomas´wife actually did so, but later retracted her appeal. Catrine’s family were considering an appeal but later claimed that the prosecutor had told them they were not allowed to file an appeal. They then tried to get permission by the Supreme Court to file a late appeal, but such permission was not granted.

16. Olsson was to be promoted Honorary Doctor by the University of Umeå for her contributions to the case. Such an honor is very rare in Sweden and is usually awarded only to outstanding scholars or to people who have supported scientific research. Olsson’s honorary degree illustrates well the climate in Swedish society at the time.

17. Teet’s young son was denied access to school until he and Teet had been tested for the HIV virus.

18. Yet, prostitution per se is not illegal.

19. Here, views differ as in so many other aspects of the case. Olsson argues that it was the specific details cited by the prosecutor that made acquittal necessary and the Court may well have convicted at least the father if the indictment had been formulated in a more general manner (Olsson, 1990). However, the incest suspicions had been thoroughly investigated and found to be insufficiently supported and Olsson’s speculations at this point seem very far-fetched.

20. The authors are very critical also of Lindeberg’s book which they find highly subjective. It is, at the same time, very easy to find their own text subjective, on the opposite side, e.g. in their acceptance of the ”testimony of the child”.

21. Swedish TV Channel 1 program, 21 December 2001, available at ”www.svt.se/granskning”. The program contains interviews with the prosecutors and some police officers who investigated the case in the 1980’s. The last person known to have seen Catrine alive was one of her clients, and his whereabouts at the time of the alleged murder were never investigated in depth. The program illustrates that Swedish media even in the end of 2001 pursue the case with great interest, in spite of what seems to be a definite closure due to the decisions made by the Supreme Court and the Supreme Administrative court earlier in that year.

 

References

Ahnlund, K. (1999, 99-01-27). Välbehövlig rannsakan av rättssamhälle i kris. Svenska Dagbladet, pp. 10-11.

Annerud, A. (1999, 2 June 1999). Styckmord utreds på nytt. Dagens Nyheter, pp. A 6.

Barnett, R. C., & Hyde, J. S. (2001). Women, men, work, and family. American Psychologist, 56, 781-796.

Bornstein, B. H., & Zickafoose, D. J. (1999). ”I know I know it, I know I saw it”: The stability of the confidence-accuracy relationship across domains. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 5, 76-88.

Bruck, M., & Ceci, S. J. (1995). Amicus brief for the case of State of New Jersey v. Michaels, presented by a committe of concerned social scientists. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 1, 272-322.

Bruck, M., Ceci, S. J., & Francoeur, E. (1999). The accuracy of mothers’ memories of conversations with their preschool children. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 5, 89-106.

Cantwell, O. (1999, 3-5 June). Articles about the cutting-up murder case. Aftonbladet.

Ceci, S. J., & Bruck, M. (1995). Jeopardy in the courtroom: A scientific analysis of children’s testimony. Washington, DC: APA.

Cutler, B. L., & Penrod, S. D. (1995). Mistaken identification. The eyewitness, psychology, and the law. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Dawes, R. M. (1994). House of cards. Psychology and psychotherapy built on myth. New York: The Free Press.

Devlin report. (1976). Report to the Secretary of State for the Home Departmental Committee on Evidence of Identification in Criminal Cases. London: Her Majesty’s Stationary Office.

Diesen, C. (1997). ? In S. Carlsson & G. Nord & L. Thorsson (Eds.), Rättsfall att minnas. Till Jan Hellner den 28 oktober 1997. Stockholm: Juristförlaget.

Diesen, C. (1997-98). Anmälan. Hellblom Sjögren, L.: Hemligheter och minnen. Att utreda tillförlitlighet i sexualmål.

Fishman, D. B. (1999). The case for pragmatic psychology. New York: NYU Press.

Goffman, E. (1963). Stigma: notes on the management of spoiled identity. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Goodman, G. S., Redlich, A. D., Qin, J., Ghetti, S., Tyda, K. S., Schaaf, J. M., & Hahn, A. (1999). Evaluating eyewitness testimony in adults and children. In A. K. Hess & I. B. Weiner (Eds.), The Handbook of Forensic Psychology (2nd ed ed., pp. 218-272). New York: Wiley.

Granhag, P. A., & Strömwall, L. A. (in press). How to detect deception? Arresting the beliefs of police officers, prosecutors and judges. Psychology, Crime and Law.

Gudjonsson, G. H. (1997). Members of the British False Memory Society: the legal consequences of the accusations for the families. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry, 8, 348-356.

Guillou, J. (1999). Lögnkampanjen om styckmordet. Folket i Bild/Kulturfront(2).

Hagen, M. A. (1997). Whores of the court. The fraud of psychiatric testimony and the rape of American justice. New York: ReganBooks.

Hellblom Sjögren, L. (1997). Hemligheter och minnen. Att utreda tillförlitlighet i sexualbrottmål. Stockholm: Norstedts.

Holgerson, A., & Hellbom, B. (1997). Facts or fictions as evidence in court. A witness psychological analysis of a Swedish legal case of alleged cutting-up murder and child sexual abuse. Stockholm: Almqvist Wiksell International.

Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1984). Choices, values, and frames. American Psychologist, 39, 341-350.

Knoff, H. M. (1998). Review of the Children’s Apperception Test [1991 revision]. In J. C. Impara & B. S. Plake (Eds.), Mental Measurment Yearbook (Vol. 13, pp. 231-233). Highland Park, J. S.: Gryphon Press.

Kringstad, H. (1997). Bjugnformelen. Oslo: Tiden Norsk Forlag.

La Fontaine, J. S. (1998). Speak of the devil. Tales of satanic abuse in contemporary England. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Levi, A. M. (1997). Are defendants guilty if they are chosen in a lineup? Law and Human Behavior, 22, 389-407.

Levi, A. M., & Jungman, N. (1995). The police lineup – basic weaknesses, radical solutions. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 22, 347-372.

Lichtenstein, S., Fischhoff, B., & Phillips, L. (1982). Calibration of probabilities: The state of the art to 1980. In D. Kahneman & P. Slovic & A. Tversky (Eds.), Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (pp. 306-334). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

Lindeberg, P. (1999). Döden är en man. Stockholm: Fischer & Co.

Melton, G. B., Petrila, J., Poythress, N. G., & Slobogin, C. (1997). Psychological evaluations for the courts: A handbook for mental health professionals and lawyers (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.

Myrdal, G., & Myrdal, A. (1934). Kris i befolkningsfrågan. Stockholm: Bonniers.

Olsson, H. (1990). Catrine och rättvisan. Stockholm: Carlsson.

Rajs, J., Lundström, M., Broberg, M., Lidberg, L., & Lindquist, O. (1998). Criminal mutilation of the human body in Sweden – a thirty-year medico-legal and forensic psychiatric study. Journal of Forensic Science, 43, 563-580.

Reinehr, R. C. (1998). Review of the Children’s Apperception Test [1991 revision]. In J. C. Impara & B. S. Plake (Eds.), Mental Measurment Yearbook (Vol. 13, pp. 233-234). Highland Park, N. J.: Gryphon Press.

Scharnberg, M. (1996a). Textual analysis: A scientific approach for assessing cases of sexual abuse. Vol. I: The theoretical framework, the psychology of lying, and cases of older children. Uppsala: Uppsala Studies in Education.

Scharnberg, M. (1996b). Textual analysis: A scientific approach for assessing cases of sexual abuse. Vol. II: Cases of younger children, including a case of alleged necrophilia, and the shortcomings of judicial logic. Uppsala: Uppsala Studies in Education.

Scharnberg, M. (1998). The defense counsel: The weakest link of the legal system in Sweden. Issues in Child Abuse Accusations, 10, 74-84.

Sjöberg, L. (1997). Ericametoden och andra projektiva test (Rapport 1). Stockholm: Psykologisk Metod AB.

Sjöberg, L. (1998). Secrets and memories: The investigation of dependability in cases of sexual abuse. Hellblom Sjögren, L.: Hemligheter och minnen. Att utreda tillförlitlighet i sexualbrottmål.

Sjöberg, L. (1998-99). Bedömningars ofullkomligheter en fara för rättssäkerheten. (Judgments errors a danger to justice). Juridisk Tidskrift, 10, 928-947.

Sjöberg, L. (2000). Projektiva test och psykodynamiskt grundade bedömningar otillförlitliga. (Projective tests and psychodynamic judgments are unreliable). Läkartidningen, 97, 56-59.

Sjöberg, L., & Montgomery, H. (1999). Double denial in attitude formation. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 29, 606-621.

Stern, E. K., & Hansén, D. (2001). Framing the Palme assassination. In J. Eriksson (Ed.), Threat politics: New perspectives on security, risk and crisis management (pp. 164-188). Aldershot, UK: Ashgate.

Stine, S. A. (1999). The snuff film. The making of an urban legend. Skeptical Inquirer, 23(3), 29-33.

Stinson, V., Davenport, J. I., Cutler, B. L., & Kravitz, B. A. (1996). How effective is the presence-of-councel safeguard? Attorney perceptions of suggestiveness, fairness, and correctability of biased lineup procedures. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 64-75.

Strupp, H. H., & Hadley, S. W. (1979). Specific vs nonspecific factors in psychotherapy. Archives of General Psychiatry, 36, 1125-1136.

Styrlin, L., & Nyberg, E. (1999). Styckmordsmålet. Stockholm: Department of Law.

Trankell, A. (1972). Reliability of evidence. Methods for analyzing and assessing witness statements. Stockholm: Beckman.

Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124-1131.

Wells, G. L., & Bradfield, A. L. (1999). Distortions in eyewitnesses’ recollections: Can the postidentification-feedback effect be moderated? Psychological Science, 10, 138-144.

Wells, G. L., Small, M., Penrod, S. J., Malpass, R. S., Fulero, S. M., & Brimacombe, C. A. E. (1998). Eyewitness identification procedures: Recommendations for lineups and photospreads. Law and Human Behavior, 22, 603-647.

Wolff, B. (1986). Fallet Alexander – ett beslagtaget barn. Stockholm: Symposium.

Vrij, A. (2000). Detecting lies and deceit. New York: Wiley.

Öhrström, L. (1996). Sex, lögner och terapi. Stockholm: Norstedts.

 

 

 

Texten återges med författarens tillstånd.